Background Image
Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  325 / 658 Next Page
Basic version Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 325 / 658 Next Page
Page Background

323

Thursday, November 10

1 4 : 3 0 – 1 6 : 0 0

a single-user medium (single, limited-size monitor, single mouse and keyboard). The gaps manifested in different acquaintance levels with the medium

(children, unlike mothers, were proficient in computer games) and perceptions regarding computer games (negative and alienated among mothers, pos‑

itive among children). The second phase included a preliminary, automatic solution whereby mothers allowed their children to assume the player’s role

while casting themselves as spectators. The third phase consisted of mother child control struggles (e.g., fighting over control of the mouse). Maternal

frustration transpired over a division of roles where she was unable to structure and lead the situation. The fourth phase was comprised of various, evolving

forms of interactions and participations, as children shared the game with their mothers through consultation, feelings shared or playing in turns. This

study examined the characteristics of adult (mother)-child interaction while playing a computer game together. Findings show that at first, this activity

raises gaps and difficulties designed to be automatically resolved in a player/spectator configuration; a configuration which must nevertheless be broken if

positive interaction is to be generated.The study suggests that a joint game which contributes to the child’s learning may require the development of digital

technologies and computer games that spare participants the default player-spectator configuration (e.g., by changing medium interfaces and contents).

Its contribution lies in the suggested model of yet-to-be-researched interpersonal interactions that play out in joint computer games. Though based on

mother-child dyads, future studies may examine this model with other dyads.